Impulsivity or patience? What does the Argentinean scientific system stimulate, and select

Authors

  • Alejandro G. Farji-Brener Laboratorio Ecotono. CRUB-UNCOma. INIBIOMA-CONICET. Bariloche, Argentina
  • Adriana Ruggiero Laboratorio Ecotono. CRUB-UNCOma. INIBIOMA-CONICET. Bariloche, Argentina

Keywords:

CONICET, human behavior, delayed gratification, trade-off, academic evaluation

Abstract

Some of the present criteria applied to evaluate the academic performance of individuals in the scientific system in Argentina (particularly, in the CONICET) confront the researchers with the dilemma of being patient to obtain larger academic benefits in the future, or rather being impulsive to get quick short-term rewards. A successful balance between these academic behaviors will determine the access, permanence and promotion of individuals in the scientific career. In this essay, we propose that although the Argentinean scientific system argues stimulating patience, it often rewards impulsivity. On one hand, the completion of doctoral theses is stimulated, but recently post-graduated students are also demanded to publish several papers in indexed journals to enter into the scientific researcher career. On other hand, the academic evaluation of young researchers gives priority to the publication of a high number of papers rather than to the supervision of students. However, then the number of supervised doctoral theses will be a limiting factor to promote to the highest academic positions. We discuss the difficulty to do these activities simultaneously, in a responsible way, and we propose some recommendations to increase the coherence of the evaluation criteria by making more coincident the academic activities that, in theory, are stimulated with those that are finally rewarded.

References

BERGSTROM, CT. 2010. Use ranking to help search. Nature 465: 870.

FARJI-BRENER, AG. 2007. Ser o no ser director, esa es la cuestión: reflexiones cómo (no) debería ser el desarrollo de una tesis doctoral. Ecología Austral 17: 287-292.

FUTUYMA, DJ. 1986. Evolutionary Biology. 2nd ed. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Massachusetts.

GARFIELD, E. 1972. Citation analysis as a tool in journal evaluation. Science 178: 471-479.

HIRSCH, JE. 2005. An index to quantify an individual’s scientific research output. Proc. Nat. Ac. Sci. 102: 16559-16572.

HOCHBERG, ME; JM CHASE, NJ GOTELLI, A HASTINGS & S NAEEM. 2009. The tragedy of the reviewer commons. Ecology Letters 12: 2-4

KORICHEVA, J. 2002. Meta-analysis of sources of variation in fitness costs of plant anti-herbivore defenses. Ecology 83:176–190.

KOZLOWSKI, J. 1992. Optimal allocation of resources to growth and reproduction: implications for age and size at maturity. Trend Ecol. Evol. 6:15–19.

KRISTAN, WB. 2003. The role of habitat selection behavior in population dynamics: source-sink systems and ecological traps. Oikos 103:457-468.

LOHELE, C. 1990. A guide to increased creativity in research -inspiration or perspiration? BioScience 40: 123-129.

MOED, HF. 2002. The impact-factor debate: the ISI’s uses and limits. Nature 415: 731-732.

NATURE PUBLISHING GROUP. 2010. Assessing assessment. Nature 465: 845.

ROSATI, A; J STEVENS; B HARE & D HAUSER. 2007. The evolutionary origins of human patience: temporal preferences in chimpanzees, bonobos, and human adults. Current Biology 17: 1663-1668.

STEVENS , J & DW STEPHENS. 2008. Patience. Current Biology 18: R756-758.

Published

2010-12-01

How to Cite

Farji-Brener, A. G., & Ruggiero, A. (2010). Impulsivity or patience? What does the Argentinean scientific system stimulate, and select. Ecología Austral, 20(3), 307–314. Retrieved from https://ojs.ecologiaaustral.com.ar/index.php/Ecologia_Austral/article/view/1310